The Experience called Colours - The Human Eye

 

Color plays an important role in the world we live in. Colors can influence the way we think, the things we feel and the way we act. They can change our mood, raise our blood pressure or even suppress our appetite. From intelligent marketing strategies to energy consumption, the application of colours is everywhere.

To understand the colourful world we live in, we must first understand our relationship with colours.

Light

Electromagnetic radiation is characterized by its wavelength and its intensity. When the wavelength is within the visible spectrum (400 nm to 700 nm), it is known as "visible light". Visible light is a small part within the electromagnetic spectrum that human eyes are sensitive to.

Visible light waves are essentially the electromagnetic waves we can see. They consist of different wavelengths. Each wavelength is a particular colour. The colour we see is a result of which wavelengths are reflected back to our eyes. Red has the longest wavelength, and violet has the shortest wavelength. When all the waves are seen together, they make white light.

Human Eye

When light hits an object, the object absorbs some of that light and reflects the rest of it. The reflected light enters the human eye first through the cornea. The cornea bends light toward the pupil, which controls the amount of light (exposure) that hits the lens. The lens then focuses the light on the retina.

The ability of the human eye to distinguish colors is based upon the varying sensitivity of different cells in the retina to light of different wavelengths. Our retina has two different types of photoreceptors (cells that detect and respond to light) — rods and cones.

Cone

Cones are stimulated in brighter environments and contain three “color-detecting” molecules that give us our colur vision. They are concentrated in the center of our retina. Each type of cone is sensitive to different wavelengths of visible light, namely:

·         L cones (long-wavelength cones or, Red Cones) - 60%

·         M cones (middle-wavelength cones or, Green Cones) - 30%

·         S cones (short-wavelength cones or, Blue Cones) - 10%

Thus, cones influence color perception and make humans trichromatic.

Rod

Rods are sensitive to light levels and help give us good vision in low light. They are concentrated in the outer areas of the retina and give us peripheral vision. This is why our peripheral vision is less sharp and colourful than our front-on vision. Rods are 500 - 1,000 times more sensitive to light than cones. It is the rods that help our eyes adjust when we enter a darkened room.

When light is bright enough, rods play virtually no role in vision at all. On the other hand, in dim light, the cones are understimulated, leaving only the signal from the rods, resulting in a colourless response.

The retina has approximately 120 million rods and 6 million cones. One could say that; while cones help the human brain interpret the hue of what we see, rods help it to interpret the contrast.

Stages of Colour Production

The experience of colour is a three stage process. It involves a light source, a spectral filter and a resolving detector.

A spectral filter is essentially an object we are looking at. Depending on the material of the object, it will selectively transmit light of different wavelengths

The human eye and the brain acts as a resolving detector. Together they translate light into color. Light receptors within the eye transmit messages to the brain, which produces the sensations of color.

Objects appear different colours because they absorb some colours (wavelengths) and reflected or transmit other colours. The colours we see are the wavelengths that are reflected or transmitted.

The surface of the apple is red because, it is reflecting the wavelengths we see as red and absorbing all the other wavelengths. An object appears white when it reflects all wavelengths and black when it absorbs them all.

Additive and Subtractive Colouring

The way a colour is produced or transmitted from a television screen is quite different from the way we see it on paper or on clothes. While one adds the wavelengths of light to produce a colour, the other absorbs the same to do so.

Additive Colouring

When coloured lights are mixed together, it is called additive mixing. Red, green and blue are the additive primary colors normally used in additive color systems. If all of these colours of light are shone onto a screen at the same time, we will see white light.

This is how TV and computer screens work. If we look closely at a screen, we would be able to see that only these three colours are being used. For example, a combination of red and green lights is used to make our brain perceive an image as yellow.

Subtractive Colouring

Subtractive coloring uses dyes, inks, pigments, paints or filters to absorb some wavelengths of light and reflect others. The color that a surface displays comes from the parts of the visible spectrum that are not absorbed and therefore remain visible.

When a pigment or ink is added to fabrics or papers, colours (wavelengths) are absorbed or "subtracted" from white light, so light of another color reaches the eye. Each time another colour of paint is mixed in, there are more colours absorbed and less are reflected. For example, if red paint is viewed under pure blue light, it will appear as black. Red paint gets its colour by scattering or reflecting, red components of the visible spectrum. If it is illuminated by blue light, it will absorb it completely, creating the appearance of a black object.

The primary colours for adding paints or dyes, are yellow, magenta and cyan. If we mix all of these colours together, they will absorb all the light and we will only see black, because no light will be reflected back to your eyes. This is also the reason a separate black ink cartridge along with the yellow, magenta and cyan ink cartridges is provided in an inkjet printer to make it cost effective.

Complementary colors

Complementary colours are pairs of colors which, when combined or mixed, cancel each other out. When placed next to each other, they create the strongest contrast for themselves. Complementary colors are also called opposite colors.

Depending on the color theory we use, the pair of complementary colours change:

·         Modern color theory uses either the RGB additive color model or the CMY subtractive color model, and in these, the complementary pairs are red–cyan, green–magenta, and blue–yellow.

·         Opponent process theory suggests that the most contrasting color pairs are red–green, and blue–yellow.

·         The black-white color pair is common to all.

This concept is known to designers and has been extensively used throughout history by artists like Vincent Van Gogh and others.

Subjectivity of Colour Perception

The experience of colour is unique to an individual. It is a feature of visual perception. But, there are various other factors which influences the way we perceive the colours around us. These factors make the relationship between the different wavelengths of light in the visual spectrum and the human experience of colour far more complex than obvious. The perception of color depends heavily on the context in which the perceived object is presented.

The same blue square appears darker in the grey region than in the green region


Vision Anomalies

Color blindness can occur when one or more of the cone types are not functioning as expected. Since there are 3 types of cone cells, there are three kinds of colour blindness. Cones can be absent, nonfunctioning or detect a different color than normal. Red-green color blindness is the most common, followed by blue-yellow color blindness. Complete colour blindness is very rare. Men are more likely to have color blindness than women.

Researchers estimate that up to 12% of females have four cone types in their retinas, rather than three. These individuals have the potential to perceive 100 times more colors than the rest of us.

Many birds, insects and fish have four types of cones. With their different cones, they can see ultraviolet light. Other animals, such as dogs, have fewer types and numbers of cones, so they may see fewer colors than humans do.

Age

Color vision can deteriorate as people get older. The lenses of the eyes become yellowish, causing older people to see everything around them in sepia tone, as if they were looking through a yellow filter. This can disrupt the "blue-yellow" vision, preventing individuals in certain situations from distinguishing blue from purple and yellow from green and yellow-green.

A cataract surgery can make major difference in such a condition because, it replaces lenses in the eyes, clearing away the yellowish film.

Chromatic Adaptation

Chromatic adaptation refers to color constancy; the human visual system’s ability to preserve the appearance of an object under a wide range of light sources. It is responsible for the stable appearance of colors of the objects around us, despite the wide variation of light which might be reflected from them and observed by our eyes. For example, a white page under blue, or red light will reflect mostly blue, or red light to the eye, respectively. The brain, however, compensates for the effect of lighting and is more likely to interpret the page as white under both the conditions.

A camera with no adjustment for light may register the white page as having varying color. When the correction occurs in a camera it is referred to as white balance.

Chromatic adaptation is one aspect of vision that may fool someone into observing a color-based optical illusion. When an artist uses a limited color palette, the human eye tends to compensate by seeing any gray or neutral color as the color which is missing from the color wheel. Here is a quick experiment:

Look first, at the image in the bottom, and fix your gaze on the dot. You will notice that the left half of the photograph has a definite bluish tone, while the right half has a definite yellowish tone. Now, fix your gaze on the dot between the blue and yellow rectangles above for about 30 seconds. After 30 seconds, shift your gaze to the dot in the image below. if you keep your eyes fixed on the dot while examining the picture, you will notice that now the tone appears same in both halves of the picture. Navigate to other parts of the images and you will notice the color differences reappearing.

After Image

Photoreceptors of a given type become desensitized after prolonged exposure to strong light in their sensitivity range. If you keep looking at a yellow light for prolonged duration, the photoreceptors in your eye responsible for sensing the yellow colour “switches-off” momentarily, as if it was bored of seeing yellow. That’s a momentary sensory deprivation. For a few seconds after the light ceases, they will continue to be in this state. Colors observed during that period will appear to lack the color component detected by the desensitized photoreceptors. This effect is responsible for the phenomenon of afterimages, in which the eye continues to see a bright figure after looking away from it, but in a complementary color.

Focus on the black dot in the yellow circle for about 60 seconds and then focus on the dot in the white circle. You will momentarily see a bluish-purplish tinge on the white circle. That’s because your eyes were tired of looking at yellow for so long.

Afterimage effects have also been utilized by artists, including Vincent van Gogh.

Synesthesia

Have you ever come across the phrase, “you can hear this picture” or “you can taste this picture” on the internet? This is because of subjective color experience triggered by input that is not even light, such as sounds or shapes. In certain forms of synesthesia, perceiving letters and numbers or, hearing musical sounds will lead to the unusual additional experiences of seeing colors, although evoked through a non-standard route.



The possibility of a clean dissociation between color experience from properties of the world reveals that color is a subjective psychological phenomenon.

Memory

Memory color is the hue of a type of object, like a banana or an apple, that human observers acquire through their experiences with instances of that type. For example, a red apple under a pure blue light should appear black to us but, our brain still perceives it as red.

Memory colors directly modulate the appearance of the actual colors of objects we see. It is not about what we see, but, what we think, we are seeing.

If we perform the after-image experiment with an inverted image of the Indian flag, the people who are acquainted with the colours and design of the India flag would find it easier to see them in the after image than those who are not. This is because they know what they are seeing and try to recreate the image from their memory.

References

https://www.sciencelearn.org.nz/resources/47-colours-of-light
https://www.olympus-lifescience.com/en/microscope-resource/primer/lightandcolor/colortemp/
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Color
https://www.olympus-lifescience.com/en/microscope-resource/primer/lightandcolor/interference/
https://www.aao.org/eye-health/tips-prevention/how-humans-see-in-color
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Memory_color_effect

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Brown Note - The Audio Laxative

Meme